gms | German Medical Science

65th Annual Meeting of the German Association for Medical Informatics, Biometry and Epidemiology (GMDS), Meeting of the Central European Network (CEN: German Region, Austro-Swiss Region and Polish Region) of the International Biometric Society (IBS)

06.09. - 09.09.2020, Berlin (online conference)

The Psychology of Biometry

Meeting Abstract

Suche in Medline nach

  • Odette Wegwarth - Max Planck Institute for Human Development Berlin, Berlin, Germany

Deutsche Gesellschaft für Medizinische Informatik, Biometrie und Epidemiologie. 65th Annual Meeting of the German Association for Medical Informatics, Biometry and Epidemiology (GMDS), Meeting of the Central European Network (CEN: German Region, Austro-Swiss Region and Polish Region) of the International Biometric Society (IBS). Berlin, 06.-09.09.2020. Düsseldorf: German Medical Science GMS Publishing House; 2021. DocAbstr. 26

doi: 10.3205/20gmds114, urn:nbn:de:0183-20gmds1149

Veröffentlicht: 26. Februar 2021

© 2021 Wegwarth.
Dieser Artikel ist ein Open-Access-Artikel und steht unter den Lizenzbedingungen der Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 License (Namensnennung). Lizenz-Angaben siehe http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.


Gliederung

Text

An evidence-based health care requires risk-savvy doctors and patients. Yet, our health care system falls short on both counts. To illustrate the extent: In a national study of 412 U.S. primary care physicians, most physicians did not know that the 5-year survival statistic – often used to communicate the benefit of screening – is a biased metric in the context of screening. Among German gynecologists who were explicitly asked for numerical information on the benefits and harms of mammography screening, not a single one provided all information required for a patient to make an informed choice. Of 32 German HIV counselors, only one were able to correctly explain the meaning of a positive HIV test result, whereas the majority claimed that a positive test result means that one has HIV with certainty. And in a study with nearly 1,700 women from 5 European countries, the majority of women overestimated their likelihood to develop certain female cancers by orders of magnitude, whereas less than one third was aware of the fact that mammography screening can also cause harms. Why do we have this lack of risk literacy? One frequently discussed answer assumes that people suffer from cognitive deficits that make them basically hopeless at dealing with statistics. Yet the fact that even 4th-graders can understand the positive predictive value if information is presented as natural frequencies shows that the problem can lie in how information is presented. Still, a transparent presentation of risk may not eliminate all of the observed misinterpretation of risk in medicine. When, for instance, over 401 US gynecologists were presented with an easy-to-understand summary of the current best evidence on ovarian cancer screening, about 48% did not revise their initial assumptions about the screening's benefit-harm-ratio, although these assumptions were incorrect. Insights from cognitive sciences imply that people's perception and dealing with risks does not only depend on how statistics is taught and communicated, but also on how medical risk information is transformed when travelling through social networks, and on how people initially learn about a risk (by description or by experience). This invited session pools together interdisciplinary forces of biometry and psychology to raise awareness for psychological and educational aspects that influences physicians' and patients' dealing with medical risks and in the consequence, the effectiveness of our health care.

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

The authors declare that a positive ethics committee vote has been obtained.


References

1.
Wegwarth O, Schwartz LM, Woloshin S, Gaissmaier W, Gigerenzer G. Do physicians understand cancer screening statistics? A national survey of primary care physicians in the U.S. Annals of Internal Medicine. 2012; 156(5): 340–9.
2.
Wegwarth O, Gigerenzer G. “There is nothing to worry about”: Gynecologists' counseling on mammography. Patient Education and Counseling. 2011; 84(2011): 251–6.
3.
Prinz R, Feufel MA, Gigerenzer G, Wegwarth O. What counselors tell low-risk clients about HIV test performance. Current HIV Research. 2015; 13: 369–80.
4.
Wegwarth O, Widschwendter M, Cibula D, et al. What do European women know about their female cancer risks and cancer screening? A cross-sectional online intervention survey in 5 European countries. BMJ Open. 2018; 8: e023789.
5.
Wegwarth O, Gigerenzer G. US gynecologists' estimates and beliefs regarding ovarian cancer screening's effectiveness 5 years after release of the PLCO evidence. Scientific Reports. 2018; 8(1): 17181.
6.
Gigerenzer G, Multmeier J, Föhring A, Wegwarth O. Do children have Bayesian intuitions? Psychological Review. Under review.
7.
Multmeier J, Gigerenzer G, Wegwarth O. How iconic representations foster children's Bayesian reasoning: studies in elementary school. In: Annual Conference of the International Mind, Brain and Education Society. San Diego (USA); 2011.
8.
Wulff DU, Mergenthaler-Canseco M, Hertwig R. A meta-analytic review of two modes of learning and the description-experience gap. Psychological Bulletin. 2018; 144(2): 140-76.
9.
MoussaÏd M, Brighton H, Gaissmaier W. The amplification of risk in experimental diffusion chains. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015; 112(18): 5631.